Tuesday, 24 July 2012

Why people participate in atrocities: mere obedience, or something more?



What makes sol­diers abuse pris­on­ers? How could Na­zi of­fi­cials con­demn thou­sands of Jews to gas cham­ber deaths? What’s go­ing on when un­der­lings help cov­er up a fi­nan­cial swin­dle?

A prevailing view, that most partici­pants in such crimes are just fol­low­ing orders, may let peo­ple off the hook a little too eas­ily, new re­search sug­gests.

Just over 50 years ago, the psy­chol­o­gist Stan­ley Mil­gram em­barked on what re­main the most fa­mous stud­ies touch­ing on the sub­ject. Par­ti­ci­pants were as­signed the role of “teacher” and were told to give shocks to some­one de­scribed as a “learn­er.” The shocks be­came stronger with each wrong an­swer from the “learn­er.” As Mil­gram fa­mously found, par­ti­ci­pants were will­ing to de­liv­er what they thought were le­thal shocks to a stranger—just be­cause ex­pe­ri­menters told them to.



Sci­en­tists have con­clud­ed based on such work that many peo­ple can’t help but obey the or­ders of those in au­thor­ity, even when those or­ders are crim­i­nal.

But not all re­search­ers agree this is the whole ex­plana­t­ion. Some sug­gest there is a more ac­tive par­ticipa­t­ion be­yond mere obe­di­ence. In an un­usu­al new stu­dy, re­search­ers Ste­phen Re­icher of the Uni­vers­ity of St. An­drews in the U.K. and col­leagues pro­pose a new way of look­ing at Mil­gram’s find­ings.

Rath­er than obe­di­ence to au­thor­ity, they pro­pose, pat­terns of so­cial iden­ti­fica­t­ion might ex­plain the ob­served be­hav­iors. Thus con­di­tions that en­cour­age iden­ti­fica­t­ion with the ex­pe­ri­menter (and, by ex­ten­sion, the sci­en­tif­ic com­mun­ity) might lead par­ti­ci­pants to fol­low the ex­pe­ri­menters’ or­ders, while con­di­tions that en­cour­aged iden­ti­fica­t­ion with the learn­er (and the gen­er­al com­mun­ity) would lead par­ti­ci­pants to de­fy the un­just or­ders.

 

Will­ing­ness to en­gage in de­struc­tive be­hav­ior may be “a re­flec­tion not of sim­ple obe­di­ence, but of ac­tive iden­ti­fica­t­ion with the ex­pe­ri­menter and his mis­sion,” the re­search­ers wrote in the stu­dy, pub­lished in the jour­nal Per­spec­tives on Psy­cho­log­i­cal Sci­ence.

Rath­er than rep­li­cate the Mil­gram stud­ies in some ver­sion—some­thing that could raise eth­i­cal com­plica­t­ions—Re­icher and col­leagues opted for an­oth­er ex­pe­ri­men­tal strat­e­gy. They took ad­vant­age of the fact that many vari­ants of the Mil­gram stud­ies have already been carr­ied out since the ori­ginal work. They re-analyzed these vari­ants, checking for wheth­er par­ti­ci­pants be­haved dif­fer­ently de­pend­ing on wheth­er the study con­text en­cour­aged iden­ti­fica­t­ion with the ex­pe­ri­menters, or with the wid­er com­mun­ity.

Re­icher and col­leagues did re­cruit hu­man parti­ci­pants, but not to press sinis­ter red but­tons. The par­ti­ci­pants were meant to serve as hope­fully im­par­tial as­ses­sors of the previous studies. Two groups were re­cruited. An “ex­pert group” in­clud­ed 32 ac­a­dem­ic so­cial psy­chol­o­gists from two Brit­ish uni­vers­i­ties and on Aus­tral­ian uni­vers­ity. The “non­ex­pert” group in­clud­ed 96 first-year psy­chol­o­gy stu­dents who had not yet learn­ed about the Mil­gram stud­ies.

All par­ti­ci­pants were read a short de­scrip­tion of Mil­gram’s orig­i­nal study and they were then giv­en de­tails about 15 vari­ants. For each var­i­ant, they were asked to in­di­cate the ex­tent to which that var­i­ant would lead par­ti­ci­pants to iden­ti­fy with the ex­pe­ri­menter and the sci­en­tif­ic com­mun­ity and the ex­tent to which it would lead them to iden­ti­fy with the learn­er and the gen­er­al com­mun­ity.

As ex­pected, iden­ti­fica­t­ion with the ex­pe­ri­menter was found to be a very strong pre­dic­tor of the lev­el of obe­di­ence dis­played, the study found, while iden­ti­fica­t­ion with the learn­er was a strong pre­dic­tor of dis­obe­di­ence. The new re­search “moves us away from a dom­i­nant view­point that has pre­vailed with­in and be­yond the ac­a­dem­ic world for nearly half a cen­tu­ry – a view­point sug­gesting that peo­ple en­gage in bar­bar­ic acts be­cause they have lit­tle in­sight in­to what they are do­ing and con­form slav­ishly to the will of au­thor­ity,” the re­search­ers wrote.

Look­ing at the find­ings this way has sev­er­al ad­van­tages, they ar­gue. First, it mir­rors re­cent his­tor­i­cal as­sess­ments sug­gesting func­tion­ar­ies in bru­talizing regimes – like the Na­zi bu­reau­crat Ad­olf Eich­mann – do much more than fol­low or­ders. And it ac­counts for why par­ti­ci­pants are more likely to fol­low or­ders un­der cer­tain con­di­tions than oth­ers, they said.

The find­ings, they added, sug­gest so­cial iden­ti­fica­t­ion gives par­ti­ci­pants a mor­al com­pass and mo­ti­vates them to act as fol­lowers. This fol­lowership, the au­thors re­mark, is not thought­less: “it is the en­deav­or of com­mitted sub­jects.”



Courtesy of the Association for Psychological Science
and World Science staff

No comments:

Post a Comment